亚色影库app

An open access publication of the 亚色影库app & Sciences
Summer 2023

Currents of Innuendo Converge on an American Path to Political Hate

Author
Norma Mendoza-Denton
View PDF
Abstract

Uses of innuendo such as enthymemes, sarcasm, and dog whistles by politicians and the resulting interlineal readings available to some listeners gave us an early warning about the type of relationship that has now obtained between Christianity and politics, and specifically the rise of Christian Nationalism as facilitated by President Donald Trump. I argue that two currents of indirectness in American politics, one religious and the other racial, have converged like tributaries leading to a larger body of water.

Norma Mendoza-Denton is Professor of Anthropology at the University of California, Los Angeles. Her research focuses on youth language, migration, politics, and identity. She is the author of Homegirls: Language and Cultural Practices Among Latina Youth Gangs (2000) and editor of Language in the Trump Era: Scandals and Emergencies (with Janet McIntosh, 2020).

The ellipsis is the punctuation of innuendo par excellence [. . .] The ellipsis points toward the moment 鈥渏ust after,鈥 inviting the reader to dwell in this blank, white, critical space so he or she may reflect on the possibility of irony within the text.    
     鈥擲rikanth Reddy1

When George W. Bush delivered the 2003 State of the Union address, Vice President Dick Cheney and Speaker of the House Dennis Hastert presided over the proceedings on the podium behind him. On that cold January evening, barely fifteen months after the World Trade Center attacks of 2001 and eight weeks before the bombing of Iraq, this speech was only Bush鈥檚 second State of the Union address and his third to both houses of Congress. Seated in the presidential box, two seats from the first lady, were some special guests: a former prostitute and drug user who now ran a heavily evangelizing church-based program to get addicts off the streets in Louisiana, representing compassionate conservatism; a former marine who repeatedly entered the Pentagon wreckage on 9/11, representing heroism and American grit; and two disgruntled physicians who had been hit by rising malpractice insurance costs, representing their own less profitable selves. Each one of these guests鈥 physical presence indexed an initiative that was addressed in the speech.2 But there were other things, a lot less overt and neither personified nor directly stated, which were in the water, 颅a escondidas鈥covertlyin the president鈥檚 speech:

For so many in our country鈥攖he homeless . . . the fatherless, the addicted鈥攖he need is great. Yet there is power, wonder-working power, in the goodness and idealism and faith of the American people. . . . I urge you to pass both my faith-based initiative and the Citizen Service Act to encourage acts of compassion that can transform America, one heart and one soul at a time.3

Political scientist Bethany Albertson probed the interpretation of the phrase 鈥渨onder-working power鈥 with an experimental setup.4 She found what was effectively an interpretive bifurcation (a dog whistle) varying in audibility according to the listener鈥檚 religious background: 89 percent of Pentecostals recognized the reference as coming from a well-known church hymn, while this effect held for only 9 percent of a more general subject population.5 Albertson additionally found that, for those who did recover the reference, a preference was exhibited for inexplicit rather than overt religious appeals, leading her to the conclusion that coded religious communication is particularly persuasive in politics.

We can corroborate these experimental results by tracing commentators鈥 reactions following Bush鈥檚 speech. The president鈥檚 supporters warmly welcomed the reference, praising the speech鈥檚 compassionate leanings, as well as an overt transfer of some of the roles of government (like dealing with unmet need among citizens) to the conditional charity of faith-based organizations. Gregory Rummo, a Christian Exchange contributor, wrote on the Writer鈥檚 Exchange Blog:

Those words will become hollow echoes as long as the obstructionists鈥攖he people who become apoplectic at the thought of God and government working in tandem鈥攎anage to block what is the only hope for the down-and-outs of society: Changed lives through the power of the Cross.6

Still others interpreted (admittedly verbally awkward) Bush 43鈥檚 role not so much as author but as animator; the words as spoken by the president were written by Michael Gerson, a fundamentalist Christian hired as a speechwriter prior to the announcement of Bush鈥檚 candidacy.7 Gerson, an opinion columnist at The Washington Post until his death in 2022, thought this was no big deal, since many presidents up until that point had hinted that they were religious, deployed mentions of God, and spoke of their faith before it became de rigeur to state one鈥檚 religious affiliation early on in the campaign.8 Additionally, when specifically asked in 2007 by journalist Kim Lawton about the idea that Bush was speaking in code to religious believers (recall some of his other [impromptu!] speeches on good versus evil, and crusades), Gerson had the following to say: 鈥淭hese aren鈥檛 code words. They are our culture. You know, millions of people understand them, and just because some people don鈥檛 get them doesn鈥檛 mean that there鈥檚 some kind of plot.鈥9

Having established that the Bush/Gerson message was on the surface about love and compassion, what motivates me to identify it as part of a downstream branch meandering toward political hate?10 And what can linguistic and discourse analysis elucidate about it? It is already well known that politicians worldwide use dog whistles in communicating with and often manipulating their constituencies.11 Rhetorical indirectness has been described鈥攊n the West at least鈥攕ince the enthymeme (in brief, a syllogism missing one of its premises), as explored by Aristotle and Theophrastus, applied to war history by Thucydides, among the Islamic philosophers by Ibn Sina/Avicenna and Ibn Rushd/Averroes, and in the East as part of Abhinavagupta鈥檚 contribution to Classical Sanskrit Rasa poetics, making meaning through Dhvani, the process of suggestion or revelation.12

American politicians鈥 interpellation of religious audiences, by indirectly indexing specific Christian beliefs on one hand and Donald Trump鈥檚 later increasingly overt invocation of eugenicist logics on the other hand, has contributed to a kind of alluvial discourse sedimentation, intensified by processes of circulation and repetition.13 The sedimentation of the detritus swirled about by these religious and racist currents provides precedent and license for even more extreme views, and has made it increasingly acceptable to 鈥渟ay the quiet part aloud,鈥 leading to our current political moment of red flags and alarm bells, constantly pinging us with instances of political hate toward non-Christians and non-whites. At the same time, an inchoate Christian Nationalist movement gains shape and momentum, churning back and forth through indexical uncertainty (our disbelieving minds have to process: Did they really just say that?), and follow-up denials of hatred and racism. Every disavowal primes the core concept. This can be seen in the exponential growth of innuendo like the ludic 鈥淟et鈥檚 Go Brandon!鈥 phenomenon described by linguist Janet McIntosh.14 It鈥檚 hard not to constantly think about an issue when everyone denies it is there, and all the denials paradoxically establish the issue as discursive common ground.15

Recent Western work in philosophy of language and the discourse/pragmatics of political hate speech has focused on 鈥渄og whistles,鈥 鈥渇ig leaves,鈥 and 鈥渟tupefying,鈥 terms that all point to the real-world effects of indirectness in the carrying out of political aims.16 Variously accounted for by processes of implicature, deniability, in-group identitarian appeals, indexical field effects, and the at-issue/not-at-issue distinction, these types of strategic conversational manipulation fall into a broader category that I will here call innuendo.17 Not only do multiple linguistic strategies involving speaker, target, audience, and interpretant support innuendo; it also happens through other semiotic channels: for instance, consider that the Trump administration鈥檚 frequent photo-ops eating KFC, while ostensibly innocuous, were a veiled sexist dig at Hillary Clinton.18 Another example is the 鈥渢ableau vivant鈥 that was Ronald Reagan announcing his presidential candidacy in the city of Philadelphia, Mississippi, the heart of the movement for 鈥渟tates鈥 rights鈥 that opposed the federal enforcement of antisegregation legislation.19

Effects such as the religious, sexist, and racist ones described above are crucially dependent on background social context: coded religious innuendo prevails in the United States because it is a normatively (though variably) secular society with an established-but-contested practice of the separation of church and state, coexisting with pervasive religiosity now bubbling forth that has until recently remained relatively excluded from official government actions.20 Along with other frowned-upon but pervasive behaviors (such as sexism, racism, and classism), this creates the conditions for religious, sexist, racist, classist, and other types of innuendo.

Consider the following now-familiar example of enthymematic innuendo as uttered by Trump, cloaked in plausible deniability, and capped off with what I have previously discussed as reactive reversal.21

Statement 8.7.2015
Then-candidate Trump, speaking to CNN鈥檚 Don Lemon, complains about Fox News correspondent Megyn Kelly鈥檚 performance at a recent presidential debate: 鈥淪he gets out and she starts asking me all sorts of ridiculous questions. You could see there was blood coming out of her eyes, blood coming out of her wherever.鈥22

Enthymematic Innuendo
Premise 1: She had blood coming out of her eyes and blood coming out of her [place called X].
Unstated Premise 2: Women menstruate out of a place called vagina. This place is unmentionable in polite society. I am being polite by not mentioning it.
Unstated Premise 2a: Because of menstruation, women are irrational.
Conclusion, to be drawn by the listener: Megyn Kelly was probably menstruating, and this made her irrational.
Possible secondarily primed conclusion: She was aggressive, like a bull seeing red (the use of 鈥済ets out鈥 and 鈥渂lood coming out of her eyes鈥).

The next day, Trump and his campaign issued two more statements, the first a tweet, the other a campaign statement attempting to rewrite his words.

Plausible Deniability 8.8a.2015
Re Megyn Kelly quote: 鈥測ou could see there was blood coming out of her eyes, blood coming out of her wherever鈥 (NOSE). Just got on w/thought23

Reactive Reversal 8.8b.2015
Mr. Trump made Megyn Kelly look really bad鈥攕he was a mess with her anger and totally caught off guard. Mr. Trump said 鈥渂lood was coming out of her eyes and whatever鈥 meaning nose, but wanted to move on to more important topics. Only a deviant would think anything else.24

Though (8.7.2015) is arguably one of the top ten most famous of Trump鈥檚 sexist statements, I want to draw attention to two aspects from (8.8b.2015). The statement was issued through a campaign press release/Twitter blast, but note the meaning-changing recasting of the prepositional phrase 鈥渙ut of her wherever,鈥 to the discourse-marking general extender 鈥渁nd whatever.鈥25 Also, the last phrase, 鈥淥nly a deviant would think anything else,鈥 is the critical piece of evidence we need to see the inner workings of how enthymemes function. The interpretation can be claimed to be dependent on the listener, and the speaker鈥檚 responsibility is thus disowned. In this case, it is not 鈥渢he corrupt media鈥 or 鈥渇ake news鈥 that promoted this interpretation. If you got that reading of 鈥渨herever,鈥 you are the deviant.

But how do we determine whether the inference was in fact invited by the statement? In conversation analysis, we apply what is called the next-turn proof procedure, looking for the interactional meaning to emerge based on how the contribution was responded to in the next speaker鈥檚 turn.26 In this case, the next turn was taken by Erick Erickson, who had invited Trump to the RedState Gathering, and who promptly rescinded the invitation, saying 鈥淚 wanted to have him here as a legitimate candidate, but no legitimate candidate suggests a female asking questions does so because she鈥檚 hormonal.鈥27 Erickson鈥檚 response is the next-proof we as analysts need to support an assertion that the original statement, in fact, carried the inference.

In 1955, sociologist Erving Goffman wrote what almost appears like a user鈥檚 manual for the kind of enthymematic innuendo President Trump was employing. It is worth quoting at length:

Tact in regard to face-work often relies for its operation on a tacit agreement to do business through the language of hint鈥攖he language of innuendo, ambiguities, well-placed pauses, carefully worded jokes, and so on. The rule regarding this unofficial kind of communication is that the sender ought not to act as if he had officially conveyed the message he has hinted at, while the recipients have the right and the obligation to act as if they have not officially received the message contained in the hint. Hinted communication, then, is deniable communication; it need not be faced up to.28

I have analyzed this type of underspecification of meaning at length elsewhere, as obtaining in pronominal forms such as something, anything, and thing, general extenders that are used in discourse precisely because they can instantiate a value that depends on the listener.29 While articulating the exact relationship between microdiscursive moves such as general extenders and broader discursive patterns of sustained political innuendo is beyond the scope of this essay, I would nevertheless like to flag this for future investigation.

Now we can turn to the remaining data for this essay, examining racist dog-whistle innuendo alluding to genetic purity (the so-called racehorse theory) from the Trump administration and its attendant troglobionts.

 

What do we want Haitians here for? Why do we want all these people from Africa here? Why do we want all these people from shithole countries? We should have people from countries like Norway.
     鈥 Donald J. Trump at a White House meeting on immigration, January 11, 2018

If you vote for me, I鈥檓 the difference, and I鈥檓 the wall. You know the wall that we鈥檙e building on the southern border? I鈥檓 your wall between the American Dream and chaos.
     鈥 Donald J. Trump at a campaign rally in Bemidji, Minnesota, September 18, 2020

One of the tributaries in my argument, racial innuendo, is illustrated by the first Trump epigraph above.30 While Trump started his presidential run by railing against Mexicans and implementing a near-total ban on travel from Muslim-majority countries, by the middle of his administration, it became clear that his 鈥渂ig, beautiful wall鈥 was largely metaphorical. The tiny, half-finished wall to the south was invoked as the means to keep out immigrants and refugees of all kinds and from all directions, but especially those who came from non-European, non-Christian backgrounds. In his own mind, as seen in the second epigraph, Trump himself was the wall.

In 2020, Trump held a rally for his reelection campaign in Bemidji, Minnesota, speaking to a largely white audience, where he began by stoking nativist fears of racialized groups, especially Muslims, and by attacking Minnesota鈥檚 Democratic Congresswoman Ilhan Omar. Remarkably, if one were reading the transcript of the speech, the first parts do not read like he is attacking Omar. However, on listening to the broadcast, we can hear the innuendo, this time in the form of verbal irony and sarcasm. This exemplifies how political innuendo includes not only veiled references to perceived flaws in an opponent鈥檚 character, or alleged groups who pose a threat to the speaker鈥檚 constituency, but also the inversion of meaning of one鈥檚 utterance through pragmatic means such as intonation. Here I provide excerpts from the rally speech for analysis. Readers can follow the link in the endnotes for the full content:31

Excerpt 1
Trump on Refugees at a Campaign Rally in Bemidji, Minnesota, September 18, 2020

Trump: (13:48) One of the most vital issues in this election is the subject of refugees. You know it. You know it perhaps better than almost anybody. Lots of luck. You鈥檙e having a good time . . . with your refugees? That鈥檚 good. We want to have . . . [turns to someone screaming in the audience]

Audience: Ilhan Omar!

Trump: Omar! He said Omar.

Aud: Boooo! Boooo!

Trump: That鈥檚 a beauty.

Aud: Boooo . . .

Trump: How the hell did SHE win the election? How did she WIN?

Aud: Boooo . . .

Trump: It鈥檚 unbelievable. Every family in Minnesota needs to know about sleepy Joe Biden鈥檚 extreme plan to flood your state with an influx of refugees from Somalia, from places all over the planet.

Aud: Boooo!

Trump: Well, that鈥檚 what鈥檚 happened, and you like Omar a lot, don鈥檛 you, huh?

Aud: Noooo . . .

Trump: Biden has promised a 700-percent increase [ . . . ] in the importation of refugees from the most dangerous places in the world, including Yemen, Syria, and Somalia. Congratulations, Minnesota. A 700-percent increase. Good luck, Minnesota. Enjoy yourselves, because if I鈥檓 not here, if I don鈥檛 win [ . . . ] Your state will be overrun and destroyed [ . . . ]

In Excerpt 1, I have inserted italics to highlight Trump鈥檚 uses of verbal irony, another type of innuendo. As devices for meaning inversion, many have described both irony and the more specific sarcasm as features of Trump鈥檚 rally delivery.32 Their commonality in part stems from a high degree of deniability. But how can we tell the utterances in question are ironic? Trump uses many rhetorical devices to signal that he means the opposite of what he is saying. He uses sarcasm (鈥淕ood luck, Minnesota鈥) and rhetorical questions (鈥淵ou鈥檙e having a good time with your refugees?鈥). Another way of generating implicatures is through the use of unexpected intonational focus.33

In Figure 1, I use the Tones and Breaks Indices (ToBI) system of intonational phonology transcription to describe the intonational patterns used by Trump to render a 鈥渟arcastic tone鈥 in his Minnesota speech.34

I鈥檝e extracted two examples below.35

1a. You鈥檙e having a good time
1b. with your refugees? . . . That鈥檚 good.

Example 1a has a high pitch accent H* on 鈥済ood鈥 and a low intonational phrase and high boundary tone L-H% on 鈥渢ime鈥 at the end of the phrase. This type of intonational contour is used to signal a continuation rise, and can be heard as a type of ellipsis. Although a transcription hardly captures it (which is why I have included the formant frequency track), this type of level high tone (see the flat visible pitch above the word 鈥渢ime鈥) invites the listener to respond, and indeed some in the audience say, 鈥淣o!鈥

In contrast, Example 1b, which features a yes/no question, would normally be expected to have a high intermediate tone and high boundary tone, H-H%, signaling a question. Instead, Trump has delivered this line with audible pauses between 鈥渞ef-u-gees,鈥 and an unexpected low pitch accent (L*) at the end of 鈥渢hat鈥檚 good.鈥 Linguists Joseph Tepperman, David Traum, and Shrikanth Narayanan have identified the narrow range and low pitch (approximately 75hz) seen in 鈥渢hat鈥檚 good鈥 as reliably signaling sarcasm in speech recognition.36 The multiple violations of listeners鈥 intonational expectations here are a strong clue that the message mustn鈥檛 be taken at face value, and that the listener must look to other, hidden dimensions of meaning.

1c. That鈥檚 a beauty.
1d. How the hell did SHE win the election?
1e. How did she WIN?

As seen in Figure 2, example 1c (That鈥檚 a beauty. H* L-L%) differs from what one would expect from a nonironic example. By putting the intonational focus on the word 鈥渢hat,鈥 and lowering the pitch for the rest of the utterance, Trump lets his listeners know that he is communicating the opposite of what he is saying. His audience responds in alignment with him by loudly booing the mention of Omar.

Contrast these unexpected occurrences (1a, 1b, 1c) with the focus given to high pitch peak accents in Examples 1d and 1e, where Trump expresses doubt about Omar having won her election.

Examples 1d and 1e occur immediately after 1c, and before each utterance, Trump resets his pitch, as is normal in English.37 He starts each intonational phrase and then produces a contrastive high pitch accent, first on 鈥渟he鈥 and then on 鈥渨in.鈥 Both of these utterances are instances of the rise-fall-rise (RFR) intonation contour: H* is the rise at sentence stress, and the low part of the utterance is the phrase tone (L-), followed by another rise at the boundary tone (H%).

The RFR contour鈥檚 meaning has been much discussed in the literature.38 Linguists Daniel Goodhue, Lyana Harrison, Yuen Tung Cl茅mentine Siu, and Michael Wagner posit its meaning as 鈥渢ak[ing] a proposition p as input, and return[ing] p as output while insinuating alternatives to p.鈥39 Thus, examples 1d and 1e, within the standard interpretations of American English intonation, yield alternative possibilities: in 1d, for other candidates to win the election; and in 1e, for Omar to lose the election. In the case of 1e, we get an incredulity reading which could be paraphrased as: She couldn鈥檛 have possibly won the election.

Trump鈥檚 alternations between observing and violating the expectations of our shared intonational grammar is part of what makes his innuendo interesting to hear for the audience, and part of what makes him a dynamic speaker. His speech is full of twists and turns, of sarcasm, innuendo, ellipsis, incredulity, and insinuations, of what sounds like in-jokes and invitations to continue his line of thought, and surely would motivate some in the audience to regard the messages as part of what sociolinguist Janet McIntosh calls alt-signaling.40

The last excerpt I will analyze reveals another device used by Trump: the dog whistle, which I define by expanding Ian Haney-L贸pez鈥檚 foundational work from 鈥渃oded racial appeals that carefully manipulate hostility toward nonwhites鈥 to also include antagonism and violence against other marginalized groups (such as discourse that encourages sexism, homophobia, anti-颅Semitism, and Islamophobia).

Excerpt 2
Trump on 鈥淧ioneers鈥 and 鈥淕enes鈥 at a Campaign Rally in Bemidji, Minnesota, September 18, 2020

Trump: (01:55:16) From St. Paul to St. Cloud, from Rochester to Duluth, and from Minneapolis, thank God we still have Minneapolis, to right here, right here with all of you great people, this state was pioneered by men and women who braved the wilderness and the winters to build a better life for themselves and for their families. They were tough and they were strong. You have good genes. You know that, right? You have good genes. A lot of it鈥檚 about the genes, isn鈥檛 it? Don鈥檛 you believe? The racehorse theory, you think we are so different? You have good genes in Minnesota. They didn鈥檛 have a lot of money. They didn鈥檛 have a lot of luxury, but they had grit, they had faith, and they had each other. [. . .] They were miners and lumberjacks, fishermen and farmers, shipbuilders and shopkeepers. But they all had one thing in common. They loved their families, they loved their countries, and they loved their God.41

Contrasting my analysis of 鈥渞efugees鈥 in Example 1b with 鈥減ioneers鈥 in this excerpt, Trump details what he thinks must have been the qualities of the ancestors of Minnesotans assembled there, qualities stemming from the genes of their presumed European pioneer forebears. I have italicized the parts of the speech I will focus on with my discussion. In the beginning of the excerpt, Trump erases the precolonization history of the state of Minnesota and of the Native peoples who live there and focuses only on the 鈥減ioneers who braved the wilderness.鈥 While praising pioneers鈥 toughness and strength, he juxtaposes the claim that the current audience has good genes, creating a causal link between the two through parataxis (they braved the wilderness; you have good genes). Next, he introduces the 鈥渞acehorse theory鈥 in what sounds like a parenthetical aside. Finally, he returns to his ongoing thought and asserts that despite all their diversity of occupation, the pioneers had one thing in common (and this part he leaves unsaid): their genes.

After this rally footage aired, outlets all across the country wrote articles and religious organizations sent protests and gave interviews alerting the public to the dangers of the overt eugenics espoused by Trump.42 The Huffington Post even compiled footage of Trump bragging about his great genes on camera. Trump biographer Michael D鈥橝ntonio shared the following observation with PBS Frontline: 鈥淭he [Trump] family subscribes to a racehorse theory of human development [. . .] they believe that if you put together the genes of a superior woman and a superior man, you get superior offspring.鈥43

 And while the mention of racehorse theory is an easily decipherable dog whistle, more sinister is the pervasiveness of Trump鈥檚 lifelong obsession with both family bloodlines and supposedly high IQ. This obsession results in his constant name-checking of his uncle who was an MIT professor, and results in absurdly challenging others to IQ tests, in boasting about his vocabulary, in bragging about his progeny鈥檚 schools, and so on. Trump鈥檚 racialized and ableist view of intelligence is in line with the reasoning for his ongoing attacks on everyone from Maxine Waters to Black athletes, and his insistence that Black people live in hell/war zones.44 Many of Trump鈥檚 callous actions against immigrants (like family separation), Muslims, African Americans, Mexican Americans, and Asians (like calling COVID-19 the 鈥淐hina virus鈥) all follow a pattern of fomenting hate toward non-whites and other targets of eugenicist movements.45

It is important to understand Trump as participating in the history of these deep-rooted racial logics. Many of the terms Trump uses descend from the legacy of John Tanton鈥檚 鈥淟atin Onslaught鈥 papers in the 1980s (Tanton was the founder of the Federation of American Immigration Reform), and at least 鈥渁nchor baby鈥 was at one point considered hate speech.46 Now it is commonplace in Trump鈥檚 speech and has even been normalized in the media.

Innuendo, whether through dog whistles, sarcasm, irony, or enthymemes, not only avoids accountability but manages to bring epistemic information into the common ground in discourse (this is why a term like 鈥渁nchor baby鈥 can become normalized). By couching divisive statements in innuendo, politicians like Trump can dodge scrutiny while still delivering sexist, racist, and xenophobic messages.

The different long-running discourse tributaries I have discussed gather speed and force to meet up at a metaphorical watershed. In just the past few months, far-right religious political figures such as Republican Congresswomen Lauren Boebert (Colorado) and Marjorie Taylor Greene (Georgia) have proudly declared themselves to be Christian Nationalists, again to the dismay of many leaders at civil rights organizations.47 These bald declarations of religious affiliation and pro-white evangelical bias would not be possible without the discourse precedent, much of it in innuendo, set forth in comments from President Trump. Christian Nationalism not only threatens the separation of church and state but has resonance with the actual Nazi-sympathizer history of the American Christian Nationalist Party, which nominated Gerald L. K. Smith in 1948, an anti-Semitic, anti-Black, pro-deportation presidential candidate with an 鈥淎merica First鈥 platform.48

Ironically, even as they protest Christianity鈥檚 ascendancy in politics, it seems difficult for American observers and media to disentangle their own Islamophobic leanings from their effort to repel racist, sexist, and anti-Semitic statements. Lauren Boebert and Marjorie Taylor Greene have both been accused of being American Taliban by thenfellow GOP Representative Adam Kinzinger (Illinois), and the members of the Supreme Court who recently overturned Roe v. Wade were roundly mocked as 鈥淎merican Taliban鈥 by media commentators, while high-profile social media accounts circulated memes of a picture altered to have most of the male judges appear to be wearing turbans and long beards, two signifiers commonly associated with devout Islamic faith, and Judge Amy Coney Barrett wearing a burka, a garment that some Muslim women wear because it covers their face and body.49 It seems even after the Trump presidency, Americans process homegrown extremism through a projection of the Other, and dog whistling once more against Muslims in the process.50

While most of the semantic and pragmatic literature I have cited aims to examine dog whistles and other types of innuendo at the level of single utterances, I argue that studying them as a historically unfolding system uncovers greater regularities and coordinated acts in messaging, as well as elucidating their support among followers and connecting individual speech acts to normalization trends and what becomes acceptable to say. I see the study of innuendo, including dog whistles, enthymemes, and sarcastic intonation, as an investigation into the pragmatics of what remains unsaid, and the recoverability of innuendo as of utmost importance for the understanding of political hate. We are all implicated, and implicated in complicity, in the making of innuendo.

Working hand in hand with other semiotic indices, understanding innuendo gives us a chance to describe the broader aesthetics of our current political moment. I hope this essay provides some tools to recognize and subvert the authority emerging from these powerful strategies while attenuating their stranglehold on discursive practices.51



author鈥檚 note

Thanks to Jill Anderson, Aomar Boum, Sandro Duranti, Janet McIntosh, David 颅Myers, Nomi Stoltzenberg, Edna Andrews, Aaron Colston, Laurie Hart, and Maggie Boum-Mendoza for inspiration and fruitful discussions. Parts of this material were presented at a colloquium jointly sponsored by the University of Chicago and University of Colorado Boulder, where I presented along with Janet McIntosh in 2021. A different version entitled 鈥淗ate and Innuendo鈥 was presented at Duke University in 2022. Flaws in this work are all my own.

Endnotes

  • 1Srikanth Reddy, 鈥溾楢s He Starts the Human Tale鈥: Strategies of Closure in Wallace Stevens,鈥 The Wallace Stevens Journal 24 (1) (2000): 13.
  • 2David Firestone, The New York Times, January 29, 2003.
  • 3George W. Bush, January 28, 2003.
  • 4 Bethany L. Albertson, Political Behavior 37 (1) (2015): 3鈥26.
  • 5 Ian Haney L贸pez鈥檚 classic work defines dog whistles as 鈥渃oded racial appeals that carefully manipulate hostility toward nonwhites.鈥 I have chosen to focus on the broader concept of innuendo partly because I want to broaden the scope beyond racism to encompass sexism, homophobia, anti-Semitic, and anti-Islamic discourse. See Ian Haney L贸pez, Dog Whistle Politics: How Coded Racial Appeals Have Reinvented Racism and Wrecked the Middle Class (New York: Oxford University Press, 2013), 21. See also Lewis E. Jones鈥檚 hymn 鈥淧ower in the Blood,鈥 1899: 鈥淲ould you be free from the burden of sin? // There is pow鈥檙, pow鈥檙, wonder-working pow鈥檙 // In the precious blood of the Lamb.鈥
  • 6Gregory J. Rummo, Writer鈥檚 Exchange Blog, (accessed August 5, 2022).
  • 7 Erving Goffman, Semiotica 25 (1鈥2) (1979): 1鈥30.
  • 8 Pew Research Center, December 6, 2004.
  • 9 Reaction to the mention of crusades is documented in Peter Waldman and Hugh Pope, The Wall Street Journal, September 21, 2001. Here is part of the text of the speech in question: 鈥淸W]e need to be alert to the fact that these evil-doers still exist. We haven鈥檛 seen this kind of barbarism in a long period of time. No one could have conceivably imagined suicide bombers burrowing into our society and then emerging all in the same day to fly their aircraft鈥攆ly U.S. aircraft into buildings full of innocent people鈥攁nd show no remorse. This is a new kind of鈥攁 new kind of evil. And we understand. And the American people are beginning to understand. This crusade, this war on terrorism is going to take a while. And the American people must be patient.鈥 Emphasis added. George W. Bush, September 16, 2001. Gerson鈥檚 quote is from an episode of Religion & Ethics: Religion & Ethics, November 2, 2007, Public Broadcasting Service.
  • 10 I will resist jumping into the topic of hate speech, for which legal status and definition vary by jurisdiction. Hate speech is neither illegal nor exhaustively defined in laws across the United States, although harassment and hate crimes are both illegal. Alexander Brown and Adriana Sinclair, The Politics of Hate Speech Laws (Abington-on-Thames, England: Routledge, 2019), 67.
  • 11 Marco Duranti, The Conservative Human Rights Revolution: European Identity, Transnational Politics, and the Origins of the European Convention (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2017); Samuel Gyasi Obeng, Discourse & Society 8 (1) (1997): 49鈥83; Joyojeet Pal, Priyank Chandra, Padma Chirumamilla, et al., International Journal of Communication 11 (22) (2017): 4197鈥4218; Alex Massie, The Spectator, February 3, 2015; and Mathilda 脜kerlund, Information, Communication & Society 25 (12) (2021): 1鈥18.
  • 12 Aristotle鈥檚 somewhat vague definition of enthymeme: 鈥渂ut when, certain things being the case, something different results beside them by virtue of their being the case, either universally or for the most part, it is called deduction here (in dialectic) and enthymeme there (in rhetoric).鈥 Aristotle, Complete Works of Aristotle, Volume 1: The Revised Oxford Translation, ed. Jonathan Barnes (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1984), lines 1356ba15鈥1356ba17. Although Aristotle referred to the enthymeme as 鈥渢he substance of rhetorical persuasion,鈥 his underspecification as to the definition of it has left much room for scholarly argument; see Lloyd F. Bitzer, The Quarterly Journal of Speech 45 (4) (1959): 399鈥408. Other scholars such as James H. McBurney have defined the enthymeme 鈥渁s a syllogism, drawn from probable causes, signs (certain and fallible) and examples. As a syllogism drawn from these materials . . . the enthymeme starts from probable premises (probable in a material sense) and lacks formal validity in certain of the types explained鈥; 鈥淚t is not essential to speak at length and with precision on everything, but some things should be left also for the listener鈥攖o be understood and sorted out by himself鈥攕o that, in coming to understand that which has been left by you for him, he will become not just your listener but also your witness, and a witness quite well disposed as well. For he will think himself a man of understanding because you have afforded him an occasion for showing his capacity for understanding. By the same token, whoever tells his listener everything accuses him of being mindless.鈥 See James H. McBurney, Speech Monographs 3 (1) (1936): 67鈥68. See also Theophrastus of Eresus: Sources for His Life, Writings, Thought and Influence, ed. and trans. William W. Fortenbaugh, Pamela M. Huby, Robert W. Sharples, and Dimitri Gutas (Leiden: Brill, 1992); Paul A. Rahe, Security Studies 5 (2) (1995): 105鈥141; Allan B盲ck, Vivarium 49 (1鈥3) (2011): 9鈥25; V. K. Chari, Philosophy East and West 27 (4) (1977): 391鈥399; and Lalita Pandit 鈥淒hvani and the 鈥楩ull Word:鈥 Suggestion and Signification from Abhinavagupta to Jacques Lacan,鈥 College Literature 23 (1) (1996): 142鈥163.
  • 13 Elinor Ochs, 鈥淣arrative,鈥 in Discourse as Structure and Process: Discourse Studies: A Multidisciplinary Introduction Volume 1, ed. Teun A. van Dijk (Los Angeles: Sage Publications, Inc, 1997), 185鈥207.
  • 14 Janet McIntosh, Anthropology News 63 (4) (2022).
  • 15 Craige Roberts, 鈥淪peech Acts in Discourse Context,鈥 in New Work on Speech Acts, ed. Daniel Fogal, Daniel W. Harris, and Matt Moss (New York: Oxford University Press, 2018), 317鈥359.
  • 16 Quentin D茅nigot and Heather Burnett, 鈥Dogwhistles as Identity-Based Interpretative Variation,鈥 in Proceedings of the Probability and Meaning Conference (PaM 2020) (Gothenburg, Sweden: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2020), 17鈥25; Robert Henderson and Elin McCready, 鈥淒ogwhistles, Trust and Ideology,鈥 in Proceedings of the 22nd Amsterdam Colloquium, ed. Julian J. Schl枚der, Dean McHugh, and Floris Roelofsen (Amsterdam:  Institute for Logic, Language and Computation, 2019), 152鈥160; Jennifer Saul, 鈥淩acist and Sexist Figleaves,鈥 in The Routledge Handbook of Social and Political Philosophy of Language, ed. Justin Khoo and Rachel Katharine Sterken (Abington-on-Thames, England: Routledge, 2021), 161鈥178; and Mike Deigan, Philosophers鈥 Imprint 22 (5) (2022).
  • 17 My own definition of innuendo is closest to Elisabeth Camp鈥檚: 鈥渢he communication of beliefs, requests, and other attitudes 鈥榦ff-record鈥, so that the speaker鈥檚 main communicative point remains unstated.鈥 Elisabeth Camp, 鈥淚nsinuation, Common Ground, and the Conversational Record,鈥 in New Work on Speech Acts, ed. Fogal, Harris, and Moss, 42. For the purposes of this essay, I consider innuendo to be the superordinate category that includes dog whistles, sarcasm, and other kinds of strategic indirectness.
  • 18 Sarah Muller, October 7, 2013, MSNBC.
  • 19 Michael Silverstein, HAU: Journal of Ethnographic Theory 7 (1) (2017): 407鈥413.
  • 20Thomas Jefferson, , January 1, 1802, The Library of Congress Archives (accessed August 5, 2022); Justice Samuel Alito, Notre Dame Law School, July 28, 2022.
  • 21 Goffman, 鈥淔ooting鈥; Adam Hodges, 鈥淧lausible Deniability,鈥 in Language in the Trump Era: Scandals and Emergencies, ed. Janet McIntosh and Norma Mendoza-Denton (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2020); and Norma Mendoza-Denton, 鈥淭he Show Must Go On: Hyperbole and Falsehood in Trump鈥檚 Performance,鈥 in Language in the Trump Era, ed. McIntosh and Mendoza-Denton.
  • 22Don Lemon Tonight, CNN, August 8, 2015.
  • 23 Donald J. Trump (@realDonaldTrump), Tweet,
  • 24 Zeke A. Miller, Time, August 8, 2015.
  • 25 Maryann Overstreet, Whales, Candlelight, and Stuff Like That: General Extenders in English Discourse (New York: Oxford University Press, 1999).
  • 26 Emanuel A. Schegloff, American Journal of Sociology 97 (5) (1992): 1295鈥1345.
  • 27 Miller, 鈥淒onald Trump Fires Back after Outrage over Megyn Kelly Remarks.鈥
  • 28 Erving Goffman, Psychiatry 18 (3) (1955): 224.
  • 29 Norma Mendoza-Denton, Homegirls: Language and Cultural Practice among Latina Youth Gangs (Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley & Sons, 2008).
  • 30 Quentin Williams, 鈥淩ejoinders from the Shithole,鈥 in Language in the Trump Era, ed. Mc颅Intosh and Mendoza-Denton.
  • 31 For a video and transcription of the speech, see Rev.com, September 18, 2020.
  • 32 St茅phanie Bonnefille, 脡tudes de Stylistique Anglaise 15 (1) (2019); Abbas Degan Darweesh and Nesaem Mehdi Abdullah, Journal of Education and Practice 7 (30) (2016): 87鈥95; and Noor Falah Hassan Akbar and Nawal Fadhil Abbas, International Journal of English Linguistics 9 (2) (2019): 113鈥127.
  • 33 For focus, see David I. Beaver and Brady Z. Clark, Sense and Sensitivity: How Focus Determines Meaning (Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley, 2009). For implicature, see H. P. Grice, 鈥淟ogic and Conversation,鈥 Speech Acts, ed. Peter Cole and Jerry L. Morgan (Leiden, Amsterdam: Brill, 1975), 41鈥58.
  • 34 The ToBI (Tones and Breaks Indices) system is an interpolation-based phonological system of annotation for intonation. ToBI was developed in recognition of the role that intonation plays in both phonological meaning and speech recognition, and taking into account that, on their own, absolute pitch values yield neither consistent percepts nor cross-speaker meaning units. The ToBI system allows for the transcription of an intonational sequence given a recording of speech and an associated spectrogram or formant record. The interpolation occurs between perceptually prominent events that can be categorized as high (and annotated H* 鈥渉igh-star鈥) or low (L*) and are known as pitch accents. Additionally, ToBI allows for compositionally derived intonational contours,  such as L+H* (鈥渓ow plus high-star,鈥 a low-leading tone followed by a high pitch accent) or H*+L (a high pitch accent followed by a low trailing tone). The most widely used conventions cover four tiers, arranged and stacked like a musical score, and labeled from top to bottom: 1. Orthographic: for orthographic words, with segmented boundaries lining up temporally with word intervals; 2. Tone: for the edges of high and low phrase tones (H-, L-) and boundary tones (H%, L%), and time values of points indicating the pitch accents, the points over which we interpolate; 3. Break-颅index: for perceived juncture/pauses; and 4. Miscellaneous: used to note disfluencies. By generating a ToBI transcription, we can abstract away from the specific details of absolute pitch value (as might result from speaker size) and temporal characteristics of talk (spoken quickly or slowly) to arrive at something more like an intonational 鈥渟ignature鈥 for a specific pitch contour, yielding a stable of pragmatic meanings within a specific variety. The ToBI system has been used to transcribe the intonation of numerous languages, including varieties of English, Spanish, French, Chinese, and Japanese, among others. Here I describe it only briefly: please consult annotation guides for fuller accounts; see Mary E. Beckman and Gayle Ayers Elam, 鈥淕uidelines for ToBI Labelling, Version 3鈥 (Columbus: The Ohio State University Research Foundation, 1997); and Mary E. Beckman, Julia Hirschberg, and Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel, 鈥淭he Original ToBI System and the Evolution of the ToBI Framework,鈥 in Prosodic Typology: The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing, ed. Sun-Ah Jun (New York: Oxford University Press, 2005), 9鈥54.
  • 35 Although examples 1a and 1b split a grammatical sentence into two parts, they are divided into two examples because each one takes place across a 鈥渂reak,鈥 that is, a perceptual juncture. We consider them as separate utterances and analyze them as such in this essay.
  • 36 Joseph Tepperman, David Traum, and Shrikanth Narayanan, 鈥溾榊eah Right鈥: Sarcasm Recognition for Spoken Dialogue Systems,鈥 Proceedings of lnterspeech ICSLP (Pittsburgh: International Conference on Spoken Language, 2006), 1838鈥1841.
  • 37 Hubert Truckenbrodt, 鈥淭he Interface of Semantics with Phonology and Morphology: Semantics of Intonation,鈥 in Semantics: An International Handbook of Natural Language Meaning: Volume 3, ed. Claudia Maienborn, Klaus von Heusinger, and Paul Portner (Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 2011), 2039鈥2069.
  • 38 Gregory Ward and Julia Hirschberg, Language 61 (4) (1995): 747鈥776; and Noah Constant, Linguistics and Philosophy 35 (2012): 407鈥442.
  • 39 Daniel Goodhue, Lyana Harrison, Yuen Tung Cl茅mentine Siu, and Michael Wagner, 鈥淭oward a Bestiary of English Intonational Contours,鈥 The Proceedings of the 46th Conference of the North Eastern Linguistics Society (NELS), ed. Christopher Hammerly and Brandon Prickett (Montreal: The North East Linguistic Society, 2016), 314.
  • 40 Janet McIntosh, 鈥淎lt-Signaling: White Violence, Military Fantasies, and Racial Stock in Trump鈥檚 America鈥 (lecture jointly delivered with Norma Mendoza-Denton as part of 鈥淭alking Politics,鈥 a colloquium delivered to and organized by University of Chicago鈥檚 Center for the Study of Communication and Society, and University of Colorado Boulder鈥檚 Culture, Language, and Social Practice, October鈥揘ovember, 2021.
  • 41 鈥淧resident Donald Trump in Bemidji, MN.鈥
  • 42 Tim Dickinson, Rolling Stone, September 22, 2020; Seema Mehta, October 5, 2020.
  • 43 Michael Kirk, dir., Frontline, Season 2016, Episode 1, aired September 27, 2016.
  • 44 Susan Currell, Amerikastudien/American Studies 64 (2) (2019): 291鈥302.
  • 45 Samuel R. Bagenstos, 鈥淭he New Eugenics,鈥 Syracuse Law Review 71 (3) (2021): 751鈥763.
  • 46 Southern Poverty Law Center (accessed July 11, 2022).
  • 47 Elizabeth Dias, The New York Times, July 8, 2022.
  • 48 Special to The New York Times, August 22, 1948.
  • 49 Josephine Harvey, Huffington Post, June 29, 2022; Jason Lemon, Newsweek, July 29, 2022; Ruth Cutler, Connecticut Mirror, June 27, 2022; and Bette Midler, Tweet, .
  • 50 Ariana Afshar, Truthout, May 14, 2022.
  • 51 Miriam Meyerhoff and Norma Mendoza-Denton, Annual Reviews of Anthropology 51 (2022): 103鈥120.