Section 2: Demographic Influences on General Views of Science
Confidence in Scientific Leaders Varies Based on Demographics and Other Factors
Percentage of U.S. Adults with a 鈥淕reat Deal鈥 of or 鈥淥nly Some鈥 Confidence in the Leaders of the Scientific Community:

Source: NORC at the University of Chicago, General Social Survey (2016). Race was self-identified through the question, 鈥淲hat race do you consider yourself?鈥 Race categories are as reported by NORC; 鈥淥ther鈥 includes all respondents, including Hispanics, who did not self-identify as 鈥淏lack鈥 or 鈥淲hite.鈥 The black bar signifies standard error.
Overall, 90 percent of Americans express either a 鈥済reat deal鈥 or 鈥渙nly some鈥 confidence in scientific leaders; this number does not significantly vary among various demographic groups. In contrast, black and female adults are less likely to report a 鈥済reat deal鈥 of confidence in scientists, while younger Americans are more likely to. Black and female adults did not report comparatively lower trust in other institutions, such as education and banks/financial institutions (data not shown).1
GSS data show that the gap between black Americans and other groups was narrower at certain points, such as the late 1980s and 2010. One recent analysis determined that inequitable educational experiences account for approximately one-third of the difference in trust.2 Other cultural elements, including religious beliefs, accounted for most, but not all, of the remaining gap. More research is needed to understand the influence of additional factors, such as the historical relationship between science and race.
Percentage of U.S. Adults with a 鈥淕reat Deal鈥 of or 鈥淥nly Some鈥 Confidence in the Leaders of the Scientific Community (continued):

Source: NORC at the University of Chicago, General Social Survey (2016). The black bar signifies standard error.
The 2016 GSS data also reveal that while 50 percent of college-educated adults have a 鈥済reat deal鈥 of confidence in scientific leaders, only 29 percent of respondents without a high-school diploma expressed this level of confidence. The positive relationship between education and trust is reversed for other institutions: people with higher educational attainment are less likely to have confidence in the military and education (data not shown).3
Higher Educational Attainment Correlates with Positive Perceptions of Science
Percentage of People Who Say that the Benefits of Scientific Research Outweigh the Harmful Results, by Educational Level:

Source: National Science Board, Science & Engineering Indicators (2018).
The NSB assessment of whether U.S. adults believe the benefits of scientific research either strongly or slightly outweigh the harmful results reveals a similar divide by education level. The 2016 data show that although 72 percent of the overall population agrees the benefits outweigh the harms, only 52 percent of people without a high-school diploma agree. Similar trends are observed with increasing family income, science knowledge, and age (although there is a dip in this perception among those 65 and older).
Percentage of People Who Agree that Science Makes Life Change Too Fast, by Education Level:

Source: National Science Board, Science & Engineering Indicators (2018).
People were also asked about the extent to which they agree or disagree with the statement 鈥淪cience makes our way of life change too fast.鈥 For the approximately 10 percent of Americans who did not complete high school, the vast majority (69 percent) agreed that science makes life change too fast.4 In comparison, 36 percent of those with a bachelor鈥檚 degree expressed this sentiment. Consistent with the strong correlation between education and income in the United States,5 respondents with lower family income also were more likely to agree with the statement (data not shown).
Trust in Scientists Varies Based on Education and Politics
Percentage of U.S. Adults Who Say They Have Confidence in Scientists to Act in the Best Interests of the Public, by Political Affiliation:

Source: Cary Funk and Brian Kennedy, The Politics of Climate, Pew Research Center (October 4, 2016; survey conducted May and June 2016). Margins of error: U.S. Adults (+/- 4), LD (+/- 7.5), M/CD (+/- 6.9), M/LR (+/- 7.7), and CR (+/- 11.4).
A majority of both Democrats and Republicans express some degree of confidence in the scientific community. But a 2016 study from the Pew Research Center found that 34 percent of liberal Democrats expressed a 鈥済reat deal鈥 of confidence in the scientific community, while only 15 percent of conservative Republicans did the same. Studies suggest that skepticism among conservatives may originate from concerns with the relationship between science and government.6
Political/Educational Breakdown of People Who Place a 鈥淕reat Deal鈥 of or 鈥淪ome鈥 Trust in Scientists to:

Political/Educational Breakdown of People Who Consider These Outcomes of Scientific Research to be an 鈥淯rgent Priority鈥:

Source: ScienceCounts, unpublished data from 鈥淩aising Voices for Science: Exploratory and Benchmarking Survey鈥 (survey conducted October 2015).
A recent exploratory market research survey from ScienceCounts included several questions to assess general public perceptions of scientists and priorities for public funding of scientific research (see pages 5 and 9). The survey examined answers based on political ideology and education level. Liberal respondents with a college degree or more placed the highest amount of trust in scientists in different scenarios鈥攕uch as reporting their findings accurately鈥攁lthough trust among all groups still remained relatively high. Conservatives tended to cluster together in their responses despite their educational background, while the responses from liberals were more varied.
With respect to research priorities, all groups highly prioritized 鈥渇inding effective treatments or cures for diseases.鈥 A higher percentage of conservative respondents considered outcomes related to 鈥渄eveloping defense and military technology鈥 a greater priority compared with college-educated liberal respondents.
Discussion and Research Considerations
Viewing perceptions of science through a demographic lens highlights the need to consider the audience when communicating science. The data support the perception that there is not one general 鈥減ublic鈥 but rather many publics who consume information based on a range of underlying factors. Moreover, social science research suggests that characterizing attitudes solely through a demographic framework provides a limited explanation of the mechanisms that inform an individual鈥檚 perspective. How people think about science varies in important ways among and within groups. In addition, while most demographic indicators are either static or subject to small shifts, perceptions change over time. A sizeable fraction of the public likely does not have strong, set attitudes about science.
Examining the Underlying Influences on Trust and Perception
People of different races, education levels, and political ideologies have differing levels of confidence in the scientific community, but the causes are unclear (pages 14鈥15). Moreover, there is a limited understanding of shared factors between groups and the governing influences behind perception. Additional research on this topic should consider the following questions:
- What causes an individual to trust science messengers? There has been research on the area of trust generally, but more research is needed that looks specifically at science messengers and the concept of scientific authority.
- How do an individual鈥檚 experiences with science shape his or her attitudes? How flexible are these attitudes once established?
- How and when do people become curious about science? Are there general trends in how attitudes and curiosity change during a person鈥檚 lifetime?
- What influence do various forms of news media, social media, and entertainment media have on trust? How do messages and cues provided by political leaders and like-minded peer groups influence trust and attitudes?
When assessing the influence of individuals鈥 experiences and worldviews on attitudes toward science, social scientists have the task of establishing a consensus on how to define fundamental terms such as 鈥渧alues鈥 and 鈥減erceptions.鈥 There is also a need for targeted research on how specific values influence specific perceptions. In line with the questions contextualizing trust in Section 1, understanding the underlying factors that shape trust in science will help strengthen science communication efforts. More-extensive collaborations between social scientists and science communicators could improve the application of these concepts to communication strategies.
Exploring Group Identity and Specific Communities
Social scientists should specifically consider how perceptions of science are influenced by group identities based on gender, race, religion, political ideology, and other factors. Historical case studies could be useful for providing insights on this topic. When considering questions of group identity, it will be necessary to consider how the definitions of the subgroups and the scientific field in question can influence the quality and interpretation of these data. It is also important not to treat these groups as monolithic, since perceptions of science may vary even among those sharing a particular group identity.
- How do we understand the role of social norms and the media in shaping cultural perceptions of science? Studies on the role of culture should explore a range of media, such as poetry, literature, television, and movies, and should evaluate possible differential effects among different subpopulations.
- What barriers to accessing science exist in low-income and marginalized communities? How does infrequent contact with high-quality scientific content affect perceptions of science?
- How do communication approaches prime a person to respond with a particular group identity? Are there communication strategies that elicit positive attitudes toward science? These questions should seek to identify the approaches that encourage discussion around shared identities and interests instead of the differences accompanying group identity.